Article Text

PDF

Cognitive function during early childhood did not improve with declining blood lead concentrations
  1. David M Fergusson, PhD
  1. Christchurch School of Medicine, Christchurch, New Zealand

Statistics from Altmetric.com

OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science

Question Are declining blood lead concentrations associated with changes in cognitive function during childhood?

Design

Cohort study with follow up from birth to 11–13 years of age.

Setting

Urban and rural communities surrounding a large lead smelter in Post Pirie, South Australia.

Participants

375 children born between 1979–82, and evaluated at 11–13 years of age.

Assessment of risk factors

Venous blood samples to assess lead concentrations were collected from the mother before birth, from the umbilical cord at birth, and from each child at age 6, 15, and 24 months and annually thereafter until age 7 years, and then again at age 11–13 years. At each blood sampling, information was obtained on a wide range of psychosocial, demographic, environmental, and biomedical factors.

Main outcome measures

Children's cognitive development assessed by interviewers unaware of the children's exposure and developmental histories using the Bayley Mental Development Index at 2 years of age, the McCarthy General Cognitive Index at 4 years of age, and IQs from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale (revised version) at 7 and 11–13 years of age.

Main results

After adjustment for confounding variables, there was an inverse association between blood lead concentration and cognitive development at all ages (ie, higher lead concentrations were associated with lower cognitive function). Mean blood lead concentrations in children decreased from 1.02 μmol/l (21.2 μg/dl) at 2 years of age to 0.38 μmol/l (7.9 μg/dl) at 11–13 years of age. Cognitive scores in children whose blood lead concentration declined most were generally not improved relative to the scores of children whose blood lead concentrations declined least (table). The analysis showed that compared with children with lower exposure levels, the cognitive deficit in the group with higher exposure changed little with age, even though blood lead concentrations declined substantially after 2 years of age.

Changes in blood lead concentration and children's IQ between 7 and 11–13 years of age

Conclusion

Cognitive function during early childhood did not improve with declining blood lead concentrations.

Commentary

This study by Tong et al may be thought of as a second generation longitudinal study of the effects of lead exposure on cognitive functioning. First generation longitudinal studies showed that at low levels of exposure, lead was associated with reductions in intellectual functioning in children after controlling for confounding factors.1 After a decade of research, the findings of these first generation studies have led to a growing consensus that at even low levels of exposure (eg, 10 μg/dl blood lead concentration), lead may have small but detectable effects on cognitive ability and academic achievement. Second generation studies have begun to concentrate on the effects of lead exposure on cognitive functioning over the life course and have established that the effects appear to extend into adolescence and young adulthood.24

Tong et al's study shows that subsequent reductions in lead concentrations do not ameliorate the effects of early lead exposures fully. Their findings clearly raise the possibility that the cognitive effects of early lead exposure are largely irreversible. Although the study is not immune to the threats to validity (eg, confounding, measurement error, sample selection bias), it makes an important contribution in showing how low level lead exposure may influence cognitive functioning over the life course.

It is important to recognise that studies in this area have been conducted in a changing environmental and political context. First generation studies conducted in the 1980s contained large numbers of participants with clearly raised lead concentrations. However, awareness of the potentially harmful effects of lead exposure has led to a decline in population lead concentrations. In effect, Tong et al have capitalised on this natural experiment. Tong et al's study raises the possibility that the effects of early lead exposure may be largely irreversible, and emphasises the continued need to minimise the exposure of young children to the potentially damaging effects of lead.

References

View Abstract

Footnotes

  • Sources of funding: National Health and Medical Research Council, Canberra, Australia and the Channel 7 Children's Research Foundation.

  • For correspondence: Dr S Tong, MBBS, PhD, Centre for Public Health Research, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, Queensland 4059, Australia. Fax +61 7 3864 3369.

Request permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.